Having Too Little or Too Much Time Is Linked to Lower
Subjective Well-Being
Marissa A. Sharif
1
, Cassie Mogilner
2
, and Hal E. Hershfield
2
1
Department of Marketing, The Wharton School, University of Pennsylvania
2
Marketing and Behavioral Decision Making Areas, Anderson School of Management, University of California, Los Angeles
Many people living in modern society feel like they do not have enough time and are constantly searching
for more. But is having limited discretionary time actually detrimental? And can there be downsides of hav-
ing too much discretionary time? In two large-scale data sets spanning 35,375 Americans and two experi-
ments, we explore the relationship between the amount of discretionary time individuals have and their
subjective well-being. We nd and internally replicate a negative quadratic relationship between discretion-
ary time and subjective well-being. These results show that whereas having too little time is indeed linked to
lower subjective well-being caused by stress, having more time does not continually translate to greater sub-
jective well-being. Having an abundance of discretionary time is sometimes even linked to lower subjective
well-being because of a lacking sense of productivity. In such cases, the negative effect of having too much
discretionary time can be attenuated when people spend this time on productive activities.
Keywords: discretionary time, happiness, life satisfaction, subjective well-being, time
Supplemental materials: https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000391.supp
Between their many obligations, people today feel like they do not
have enough time and want more. This time famine reaches across
the globe (Hamermesh & Lee, 2007) and is particularly acute among
Americans, who report feeling more time-constrained than ever before
(Carroll, 2008; Roxburgh, 2004). A nationwide poll shows that nearly
half of Americans report they do not have enough time to do what they
want to do (Newport, 2016). Hoping to lessen the strain, many people
search for ways to save time to increase hours spent doing what they
want. For i nstance, there a re more than 35,000 books available on
Amazon aiming to improv e time management (e.g., How to Get 12
Hours Out of an 8-Hour Day), and 50% of Americans spend money to
buy out of chores, such as cooking, shopping, and household mainte-
nance (Whillans et al., 2017). Although prioritizing time over money
and spending money to buy more free time have been empirically
linked to greater happiness (Hersheld et al., 2016; Whillans et al.,
2017, 2016), would actually having more time make people better
off? Pushing this question further, is it possible to have too much dis-
cretionary time? Across two large-scale data sets and two experiments,
we examine the relationship between the amount of discretionary time
people have and their subjective well-being.
The likely harm of having too little time is straightforward. People
who work longer hours and have a greater proportion of their sched-
ules consumed by obligations have less time to do what they want.
Less discretionary time means less time spent on activities that are
linked to greater happiness (e.g., socializing and engaging in active
leisure; Csikszentmihalyi & Hunter, 2003; Kahneman et al., 2004;
Lathia et al., 2017; Mogilner, 2010). In addition, feeling pressed for
time takes its toll. People who report greater feelings of time-stress
exhibit more unhealthy behaviors, such as eating poorly and not
exercising (Banwell et al., 2005; Strazdins et al., 2011). Those who
report feeling time-stressed also report being less happy (Kasser &
Sheldon, 2009; Masuda et al., 2020), more depressed (Roxburgh,
2004), and more emotionally exhausted (Teuchmann et al., 1999).
Although a negative effect of being temporally impoverished
seems likely, what is the effect of having an abundance of time?
Ample time for discretionary activities may have a diminishing
effect on peoples enjoyment of those activities. Both fortunately
and unfortunately, people are prone to hedonic adaptation, making
them grow accustomed not only to lifes pains but also to lifespleas-
ures (Frederick & Loewenstein, 1999; Lyubomirsky et al., 2005;
Wilson & Gilbert, 2008). For instance, despite socializing being
among the most enjoyable ways to spend time, time spent on social
activities has been shown to have diminishing returns for subjective
well-being (Kushlev et al., 2018). Indeed, excessive access to
enjoyed activities leads people to savor them less (Kurtz, 2008;
Quoidbach et al., 2015). Therefore, spending hours upon hours sim-
ply doing what one wants may lose its positive impact on happiness.
Beyond a reduced positive effect, could there ever be a negative
effect? Is it possible to have too much discretionary time? Are there
cases in which having additional discretionary hours is associated
Marissa A. Sharif https://orcid.org/0000-0001-7910-7000
OSF Link for data, syntax, materials, and preregistrations: https://osf.io/
hw85m/.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Marissa
A. Sharif, Department of Marketing, The Wharton School, University of
Pennsylvania, 3730 Walnut Street, Jon M. Huntsman Hall 751,
Philadelphia, PA 19103, United States, or to Cassie Mogilner, Marketing
and Behavioral Decision Making Areas, Anderson School of Management,
University of California, Los Angeles, 110 Westwood Plaza, Gold Hall B-
515, Los Angeles, CA 90095, United States. Email: masharif@wharton
.upenn.edu or [email protected]
1
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology:
Personality Processes and Individual Differences
© 2021 American Psychological Association
ISSN: 0022-3514 https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000391
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
with lower subjective well-being? Emerging work tangentially sug-
gests so. People dread being idle and have been observed as happier
when busied by a task (Hsee et al., 2010). Recent research suggests
that busyness has become a status symbol, signaling competence,
ambition, and being in high demand (Bellezza et al., 2017). If people
derive satisfaction from being productive with their time (i.e., spend-
ing it on worthwhile activities; Keinan & Kivetz, 2011), the effect of
having an abundance of time in ones daily life may be more insidi-
ous than mere boredom. With too much discretionary time, people
may infer lack of productivity and purpose (Csikszentmihalyi, 2000)
thus feeling less happy and less satised in their lives.
Even though many people in modern society feel time-stressed
and want more time, we hypothesized that actually having more
discretionary time would not increasingly make people better off.
More specically, we predicted a negative quadratic relationship
between discretionary time and subjective well-being, such that
beyond a certain amount, more discretionary time would not be
further associated with greater subjective well-being. We further
expected that in some cases, a large amount of discretionary time
may actually be associated with lower subjective well-being,
depending on how this time is spent. In particular, we predicted
that an abundance of time spent on nonproductive discretionary
activities would exhibit a negative effect on subjective well-being.
However, if people spent their discretionary time in productive
ways, we predicted that the negative effect of having too much dis-
cretionary time would be attenuated.
Drawing on prior research (Holbrook & Lehmann, 1981), we
dene discret ionar y time as the amount of time spent on leisur e
activities or other pursuits where the primary function is the use of
time for pleasure or another intrinsicall y worthwhile purpose. There-
fore, discretiona ry time is not simply whatever time remains outside
of paid work hours. For one, not everyone works for pay (e.g., stay-
at-home parents and retirees). And among those who do, the amount
of time one chooses to spend working might be well over what is
obligatory (Snir & Zohar, 2008). Additionally, at least some of ones
time outside of work is likely to be absorbed by other obligations,
including household chores, going to the dentist, taking a child to the
dentist, or standing in line at the DMV
1
(Eriksson et al., 2007). Thus,
we dene and operationalize discretionary time as the number of
hours a person spends in a day doing what they want.
To examine the relationship between discretionary time and subjec-
tive well-being, we rst analyze two large-scale data sets representing
adults from across the United States (Studies 1 and 2). We then sup-
plement these data with two mental simulation experiments to gain
insight into mechanisms driving the observed effect. In particular, in
Study 3, we measure two theoretically motivated mediators: greater
stress from having too little discretionary time and a lacking sense of
productivity from having too much discretionary time. In Study 4, we
then manipulate whether a high (vs. moderate) amount of discretion-
ary time is spent on product ive (vs. nonprod uctive ) activities to test
for an attenuation of the negative effect of having too much time.
Study 1: National Study of the Changing Workforce
Method
We analyzed the data of 13,639 working Americans who partici-
pated in the National study of the Changing Workforce (NSCW)
between 1992 and 2008.
2,3
The NSCW surveyed representative sam-
ples of the nations labor force on four occasions: in 1992; in 1997; in
2002; and in 2008 (ages 1899, M
age
= 42.27; 47.2% male; 78.8%
Caucasian; 58.1% married; 41.7% have children; 36.9% with at least
a bachel or's degree; M
income
= $46,398.47). To qualify to take this sur-
vey, par ticip ants had to be working at a paid jo b or operating an
income-producing business as part of the civilian labor force (see
Galinsky et al., 1993 for more information about this survey).
Among the surveys many questions, participants reported their
amount of discretionary time (On average, on days when youre
working, about how many hours [minutes] do you spend on your own
free-time activities?; M =1.80hr,SD = 1.82, Median = 1 hr, Min =
0 hr, Max = 20 hr; see Figure S1 for distribution) and their subjective
well-being, which was measured as life satisfaction (All things con-
sidered, how do you feel about your life these days? Would you say
you feel 1 = very satised ,2=somewhat satised,3=somewhat dis-
satised,or4=very dissatised?). For our analysis, we reverse-
coded the life satis factio n measure, such that larger numbers corre-
spond with greater subjective well-being (M =3.24,SD =.71).
Whereas many papers have examined this data set, https://www
.familiesandwork.org/research/workforce-research-national-study-of
-the-changing-workforce, research-to-date has not examined the
relationship between the amount of discretionary time people have
and their life satisfaction.
Results
We conducted an OLS regression predicting life satisfaction
from the linear and quadratic terms of peoples reported amount of
discretionary time. Consistent with our prediction, we found a sig-
nicant negative quadratic relationship between the amount of dis-
cretionary time people have and their subjective well-being (N =
13,639, B = .003, SE = .001, t[13636] = 5.28, p , .001, 95%
CI [.004, .002], R
2
= .004; Figure 1, Table 1).
We also examined whether the quadratic term explained more
variance in the model than did the signicant linear term alone (N =
13,639, B =.017,SE =.003,t[13639] = 5.11, p , .001, 95% CI
[.011, .024], R
2
= .002). Indeed, by adding the quadratic term in the
model, there was a signicant increase in the variance explained: an
increase of .002 in the R
2
, F Change (1, 13636) = 27.85, p , .001.
The predicted negative quadratic relationship held when con-
trolling for gender, age, parental status, marital status, race, the
natural log of respondent income, employment status (i.e.,
self-employed or not), and education
4
(N = 11,649, B = .003,
SE = .001, t[11638] = 3.72, p , .001, 95% CI [.011, .004],
R
2
= .058; Table 1). We did not nd any consistent interactions
between these covariates and the predicted quadratic relationship
across studies.
1
Other than work, these activities were among those viewed as least
discretionary (and most obligatory) in the crowdsourcing study we
conducted to determine our measure of discretionary time for Study 2. See
Table S7 in the online supplemental materials for the complete set of
activities listed from most to least discretionary.
2
Studies 1 and 2 analyzes large datasets collected by a third party with
no identifying information about the participants; thus, IRB approval was
not necessary for these studies.
3
Data are not publicly available.
4
Gender, age, parental status, marital status, race, education, income,
and self-employment status are all mean-centered.
2
SHARIF, MOGILNER, AND HERSHFIELD
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Outliers
The predicted negative quadratic effect held up to a series of
robustness checks. Namely, the effect persisted when we excluded
individuals who reported discretionary time four or more standard
deviations away from the mean (n = 108 with 9.1þ hours of discre-
tionary time; without covariates: N = 13,531, B = .010, SE =.002,
t[13528] = 6.02, p , .001, 95% CI [.014, .007], R
2
=.006;
with covariates: N =11,564,B = .010, SE =.002,t[11553] =
3.39, p , .001, 95% CI [.023, .006], R
2
=.060;Table S1).
The effect also persisted when we excluded individuals who were
identied as outliers using Cooks Distance (without covariates: n =
290 with Cooks distance greater than 4/N, N = 13,349, B = .004,
SE =.001,t[13346] = 5.75, p , .001, 95% CI [.005, .003], R
2
= .007; with covariates: n = 436 with Cooks distance greater than
4/N, N =11,213,B = .004, SE =.001,t[11202] = 4.61, p ,
.001, 95% CI [.005, .002], R
2
=.072;Table S2). These checks
conrm that the observed negative quadratic effect was not driven
by a few participants who reported having very large amounts of
discretionary time.
U-Shape Test
We then used Simonsohns (2018) two-lines approach (http://
webstimate.org/twolines/) to test for an inverted U-shape in the
relationship between discretionary time and subjective well-being.
This method conrmed that for low values of discretionary time,
the regression line was positive and statistically signicant (b =
.090, z = 9.291, p , .001); however, for high values of discretion-
ary time, the regression line was negative but did not reach statisti-
cal signicance ( b = .005, z = .899, p = .368; break point = 2
hr). Thus, as we predicted, having more discretionary time did not
show a continued positive effect on subjective well-being. How-
ever, in this dataset, we did not observe our predicted signicant
negative effect among people who had an abundance of discretion-
ary time. In light of our results from Study 2, we later propose
why we likely did not observe the predicted drop in this dataset.
Figure 1
Scatterplot of the Relationship Between Discretionary Time and Subjective Well-Being in Study 1
1
2
3
4
0 5 10 15 20
Subjective Well-Being
Discretionary Hours
Note. For ease of visualization, a jitter was added to subjective well-being scores and discretionary hours. Fit
line represents the negative curvilinear relationship between discretionary time and subjective well-being.
Table 1
Regression Results of Study 1: The Influence of Discretionary
Time on Subjective Well-Being
Variable (1) (2)
Hours of discretionary time 0.044*** 0.057***
(0.006) (0.007)
Hours of discretionary time squared 0.003*** 0.003***
(0.001) (0.001)
Male 0.025
þ
(0.013)
White 0.012
(0.016)
Age 0.002*
(0.001)
Married 0.293***
(0.014)
Children 0.034*
(0.015)
4-year college 0.034*
(0.014)
Natural log-transformed income 0.047***
(0.007)
Self-employed 0.078***
(0.018)
Constant 3.18*** 3.15***
(.010) (.011)
R
2
.004 .058
Note. (1) The coefficients reported above are the unstandardized coeffi-
cients. (2) All predictor variables, except Hours of Discretionary time and
Hours of Discretionary time Squared, are mean-centered.
þ
p , .10. * p , .05. ** p , .01. *** p , .001.
DISCRETIONARY TIME AND SUBJECTIVE WELL-BEING
3
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Study 2: American Time Use Survey
The dataset we analyzed in Study 2 advances our investigation
in several important ways. First, Study 1 only included working
Americans. In Study 2, we leveraged data from an even larger and
more representative sample of Americans, including those not in
the workforce.
Second, Study 1 relied on a fairly subjective measure of discre-
tionary time. Participants used their own interpretation of what
constitutes free-time ac tivities to report their amount of discre-
tionary time on an average workday. However, an activity that
one person views as discretionary (e.g., going for a run) might be
viewed as a tortuous chore by someone else, or an activity that
someone views a s a tedious, incessant obligation (e.g., cooking)
might be viewed as an enriching hobby by others. Although the
results of Study 1 were informative in capturing individuals
views of th eir activities in a typical day, in Study 2 we sought to
replic ate the predicted negative quadratic effect using a more
objective and conservative measure of discretionary time. Based
on the activities that most pe ople (i.e., more than 90%) view as
discretionary, we calculated the amount of time e ach of the tens
of thousands of individuals in the dataset spent on discretionary
activities in a given day. This level of detail in the dataset allowed
us to further explore the role that different types of discretionary
activities play in affecting the relationship between the amount of
discretionary time individuals have and their subjective well-
being.
Method
We analyzed the data of 21,736 Americans who participated in
the American Time Use Survey (ATUS) between 2012 and 2013,
the years in which our key variables were administered (ages
1585, M
age
= 47.92; 44.5% male; 79.3% Caucasian; 47.7% mar-
ried; 43.5% have children; 33.5% with at least a bachelor's degree;
57.8% employed full time; M
income
= $52,597.74). Data are avail-
able at: https://www.bls.gov/tus/#database. In answering the
ATUS, respondents provide a detailed account of the activities
that lled their prior 24 hrindicating the time period and dura-
tion of each activity. We assessed discretionary time by calculat-
ing the amount of time each individual spent on discretionary
activities in a day.
Because there is no standard denition for which specic activ-
ities count as discretionary, we used a crowdsourcing platform
(Amazons Mechanical Turk) to determine the activities that most
people (i.e., more than 90%) consider to be discretionary time.
Specically, we conducted a preregistered study in which we rst
provided a denition of discretionary time (time spent on leisure
activities or other pursuits where the primary function is the use of
time for pleasure or some other intrinsically worthwhile purpose)
and then asked participants (N = 500; M
age
= 34.59, SD
age
= 10.83;
50.8% male) to indicate for each activity in the American time
Use Survey whether it was representative of discretionary time or
not (0 = is NOT discretionary time, 1=IS discretionary time).
The categories of activities that at least 90% of the sample consid-
ered discretionary were Relaxing & Leisure (e.g., doing nothing,
watching TV, listening to the radio, playing games); Socializing &
Communicating with Others (e.g., hanging out with family, hang-
ing out with friends); Arts & Entertainment Other than Sports
(e.g., attending a comedy club, attending an art gallery, attending a
movie); Travel Related to Socializing, Relaxing, & Leisure; Perso-
nal Activities (e.g., having sex, making out); Attending Sporting/
Recreational Events (e.g., watching sports); Playing Sports with
Household and Nonhousehold Children (e.g., riding bikes with
child, strolling with child); and Participating in Sports, Exercise,
or Recreation (e.g., biking, playing basketball, shing, running,
golng, doing yoga, working out). See Table S7 of the online sup-
plemental materials for more details about Study 2, including the
full list of 139 activities and the percentage of participants who
identied each as discretionary.
Applying this crowdsourced metric for determining which
activities count as discretionary, we calculated the total amount
of time each American time Use Survey respondent spent engag-
ing in discretionary activities over the previous 24 hr (M =5.48
hr, SD =3.70,Median=4.92hr,Min=0hr,Max=23.98hr;see
Figure S2 for the distribution).
5
Subjective well-being was
assessed with a ladder-style question used in prior research
(Kahneman & Deaton, 2010): Please imagine a ladder with
steps numbered from 0 at the bottom to 10 at the top. The top of
the ladder represents the best possible li fe for you. If the top step
is 10 and the bottom step is 0, on which step of the ladder do you
feel you per sonally stand at the present time? (M = 7.11, SD =
2.03). In doing so, unlike previous research examining this data,
https://www.bls.gov/tus/research.htm, we were able to uniquely
examine how amount of time spent on discretionary acti vities
affects subjective well-being.
Results
We conducted an OLS regression predicting life satisfaction
from the linear and quadratic terms of our calculated amount of
individuals discretionary t ime. Replicat ing the r esults from
Study 1, we found a signicant negative quadratic relationship
between the amount of discretionary time people have and their
subjective well-being (N = 21,736, B = .004, SE = .001,
t[21733] = 4.88, p , .001, 95% CI [.005, .002], R
2
= .003;
Figure 2 and Table 2). This effect held when controlling for gen-
der, age, parental status, marital status, race, education, natural
log-transformed respondent income, and employment status, all
of which are mean-centered ( N = 20,275, B = .003, SE = .001,
t[20264] = 4.23, p , .001, 95% CI [.005, .002], R
2
= .032;
Table 2).
Inclusion of the predicted negative quadratic term explained
more variance in the model than did the linear term alone, which
contrary to Study 1 was signicant and negative (N = 21,736, B =
.026, SE = .004, t[21734] = 6.89, p , .001, 95% CI [.033,
.018], R
2
= .002). By adding the quadratic term in the model,
there was a signicant increase in the variance explained: an
increase of .001 in the R
2
, F Change (1, 21733) = 23.77, p , .001.
5
It is apparent in Figure S2 that a small portion of respondents had a
very large number of discretionary hours in their day. For instance, 5.9% of
the sample had more than 12 hours of discretionary time. Although it may
seem difcult to have this large amount of discretionary time in a day, it is
not unreasonable. The ATUS asks individuals about a randomly selected
day, which might be one in which the person had an unusually large
number of discretionary hours (with little sleep and/or few obligations)
4
SHARIF, MOGILNER, AND HERSHFIELD
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Robustness Checks
The predicted negative quadratic effect held up to a series of
robustness checks.
Outliers
The effect held when we excluded individuals who reported dis-
cretionary time four or more standard deviations away from the
mean (n = 13 with 20.28þ hours of discretionary time; without
covariates: N = 21,723, B = .004, SE = .001, t[21720] = 4.42,
p , .001, 95% CI [.005, .002], R
2
= .003; with covariates: N =
20,262, B = .003, SE = .001, t[20251] = 3.76, p , .001, 95%
CI [.005, .002], R
2
= .031; Table S3). It also held when we
excluded outliers using Cooks Distance (n = 1,088, without cova-
riates: N = 20,648, B = .003, SE = .001, t[20645] = 3.21, p =
.001, 95% CI [.005, .001], R
2
= .001; n = 858, with covariates:
N = 19,417, B = .002, SE = .001, t[19406] = 3.01, p = .003,
95% CI [.004, .001], R
2
= .036; Table S4). These results sug-
gest that the predicted effect is not reliant on the inclusion of out-
liers who had an extremely large amount of discretionary time
that day.
Discretionary Time Calculations
Further, the effect held when we loosened the restriction of
what constitutes a discretionary activity from those activities iden-
tied by at least 90% of participants as discretionary to those that
at least 75% of participants identied as discretionary (M = 6.08
hr, SD = 3.76, Median = 5.62 hr, Min = 0 hr, Max = 23.98 hr;
without covariates: N = 21,736, B = .004, SE = .001, t[21733] =
5.56, p , .001, 95% CI [.006, .003], R
2
= .003; with covari-
ates: N = 20,275, B = .004, SE = .001, t[20264] = 4.57, p ,
.001, 95% CI [.005, .002], R
2
= .031; Table S5).
The effect also held when we used an alternate measure of dis-
cretionary time. For this, we calculated the total amount of time
respondents spent doing any activity that fell within the American
time Use Surveyspredened category of Socializing, Relaxing,
and Leisure (e.g., socializing and communicating with others,
Table 2
Regression Results of Study 2: The Influence of Discretionary
Time on Subjective Well-Being
Variable (1) (2)
Hours of discretionary time 0.027* .035
(0.011) (0.012)
Hours of discretionary time squared 0.004*** 0.003***
(0.001) (0.001)
Male 0.189***
(0.029)
White 0.051
(0.035)
Age 0.006***
(0.001)
Married 0.577***
(0.031)
0.076*
Children (0.036)
0.117***
(0.032)
4-year college 0.030***
(0.008)
Natural log-transformed earnings 0.279**
Employed (0.087)
Constant 7.134*** 7.070***
(0.034) (0.037)
R
2
.003 .032
Note. (1) The coefficients reported above are the unstandardized coeffi-
cients. (2) All predictor variables, except Hours of Discretionary time and
Hours of Discretionary time Squared, are mean-centered.
þ
p , .10. * p , .05. ** p , .01. *** p , .001.
Figure 2
Scatterplot of the Relationship Between Discretionary Time and Subjective Well-Being in Study 2
0
2
4
6
8
10
0 5 10 15 20
Subjective Well-Being
Discretionary Hours
Note. For ease of visualization, a jitter was added to subjective well-being scores. Fit line represents the nega-
tive curvilinear relationship between discretionary time and subjective well-being.
DISCRETIONARY TIME AND SUBJECTIVE WELL-BEING
5
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
attending or hosting social events, etc.). Using this alternative mea-
sure, we again observed the predicted negative quadratic relation-
ship (M =5.04hr,SD = 3.57, Median = 4.42 hr, Min = 0 hr, Max =
23.98 hr; without covariates: N =21,736,B = .004, SE =.001,t
[21733] = 4.64, p , .001, 95% CI [.006, .002], R
2
=.005;
with covariates: N = 20,275, B = .003, SE =.001,t[20264] =
3.91, p , .001, 95% CI [.005, .002], R
2
=.033;Table S6).
Working Versus Not
Whereas Study 1 only included working Americans, Study 2
included individuals who were working (n = 12,558) as well as
those who were not working (n = 9,178), including those who
were employed but absent from work that day (n = 543), those
who were unemployed (n = 1,159), and those not in the labor force
(n = 7,476). Even though those who were working had less discre-
tionary time on average (M = 4.39 hr, SD = 3.20) than those who
were not working (M = 6.96 hr, SD = 3.82), we found the pre-
dicted negative quadratic relationship between individuals
amount of discretionary time and subjective well-being both
among workers (N = 12,558, B = .003, SE = .001, t[12555] =
2.23, p = .025, 95% CI [.005, .000], R
2
= .001) and non-
workers (N = 9,178, B = .004, SE = .001, t[9175] = 3.43, p =
.001, 95% CI [.007, .002], R
2
= .005). As additional evidence
that the effect was not driven by people who were involuntarily
unemployed and thus whose abundance of discretionary time was
potentially unwelcomed and uncoordinated with their social net-
work (Young & Lim, 2014), we still observed the signicant nega-
tive quadratic effect when we excluded only individuals who
self-identied as being unemployed (excluded n = 1,159;
M
Discretionary time of Unemployed Participants
= 6.40 hr, SD = 3.77; N =
20,576, B = .004, SE = .001, t[20574] = 5.03, p , .001, 95%
CI [.006, .003], R
2
= .003).
We also found that the effect was not driven by retirees. As evi-
dence, the predicted negative quadratic relationship persisted when
we excluded individuals who were above the standard retirement
age of 66 (excluded n =3,709;M
Discretionary time of individuals over 66
=
7.50 hr, SD =3.65;N =18,027,B = .004, SE =.001,t[18024] =
4.61, p , .001, 95% CI [.006, .002], R
2
=.007).Furthermore,
whether or not an individual was above the standard age of retire-
ment or not did not moderate the negative quadratic term: the inter-
action effect was not signicant (B =.000,SE =.002,t[20261] =
.198, p =.843,95%CI[.005, .004]).
Weekday Versus Weekend
Further, even though people on average have more discretionary
time on the weekends (n = 11,039; M =6.16hr,SD = 3.71) than on
the weekdays (n = 10,697; M =4.77hr,SD = 3.56), the negative
quadratic effect persisted among people reporting their discretionary
activities either on a weekend (N = 11,039, B = .004, SE = .001, t
[11036] = 3.98, p , .001, 95% CI [.006, .002], R
2
= .003) or
on a weekday (N = 10,697, B = .004, SE = .001, t[10694] = 3.21,
p = .001, 95% CI [.006, .001], R
2
= .004). This analysis suggests
that people can experience too little time and too much discretionary
time irrespective of whether its during the week or on the weekend.
U-Shape Test
We again used Simonsohns (2018) two-lines procedure to test
for an inverted U-shape relationship between discretionary time
and subjective well-being. Here, the regression line for low values
of discretionary time was again positive and statistically signi-
cant (b = .068, z = 3.105, p = .002). However, unlike in Study 1,
the regression line for the high values of discretionary time was
negative and statistically signicant, as we had initially predicted
(b = .041, z = 6.657, p , .001; break point = 3.42 hr). That is,
in this dataset, we again found evidence for having too little time;
at the low end of the continuum for discretionary time, having
more time was associated with greater subjective well-being.
However, we also found evidence for having too much time; at the
high end of the continuum for discretionary time, having more
time was associated with lower subjective well-being.
For a clearer view into the results, we supplemented this analy-
sis with a histogram of respondents amount of discretionary time
binned per 30 minutes. Based on Figure 3, it appears that subjec-
tive well-being increases as discretionary time increases between
zero and two hours, peaks between two and ve hours, and
decreases above ve hours. These results provide a rough approxi-
mation that having less than two hours of discretionary time is
too little, whereas having more than ve hours of discretionary
time is too much.
This identication of what counts as too little or too much dis-
cretionary time offers a potential clue into why we did not observe
the too much effect in Study 1. Although both studies showed a
signicant negative quadratic effect, the U-shape test in Study 1
showed that the positive relationship between discretionary time
and life satisfaction leveled off after the break point, whereas the
U-shape test in Study 2 showed that the relationship became sig-
nicantly negative after a point. If having more than ve hours of
discretionary time counts as too much, we see that while 48.1%
of participants in Study 2 qualied as having too much, only 3.5%
of participants in Study 1 did. See Figure S1 and Figure S2 in the
online supplemental materials for the distribution of discretionary
time in each study. The very small proportion of individuals in
Study 1 with too much discretionary time may not have provided
enough power to detect a signicant negative effect on subjective
well-being from having an overabundance of discretionary time.
Yet another difference between the two studies was the measure
for discretionary time. Whereas Study 1 asked participants to
report their amount of discretionary time on average, in Study 2
we calculated the amount of time people actually spent on discre-
tionary activities on a given day. With its larger dataset that repre-
sents individuals both in and out of the workforce, and its more
precise and objective measure of discretionary time, we suggest
that the pattern of results found in Study 2 is more conclusive.
Types of Discretionary Time
With the robustness of the predicted negative quadratic effect,
along with the signicant test conrming the inverted U-shape, the
results of Study 2 suggest that not only is it possible to have too lit-
tle discretionary time, it is also possible to have too much. We
found that having an abundance of discretionary time is associated
with lower subjective well-being. We then wondered whether the
way people spent their discretionary time might affect this relation-
ship. That is, if people were to spend their discretionary time in
more worthwhile ways, we may see the too much time effect atte-
nuated. For instance, prior research has identied socializing and
active leisure as particularly positive uses of time (Csikszentmihalyi
6
SHARIF, MOGILNER, AND HERSHFIELD
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
& Hunter, 2003; Kahneman et al., 2004; Mogilner et al., 2018).
Research has also shown that being productive with ones time pro-
duces greater subjective well-being (Etkin & Mogilner, 2016;
Keinan & Kivetz, 2011). To explore whether the type of discretion-
ary time moderates the relationship between amount of discretion-
ary time and subjective well-being, we reexamined the data from
the ATUS. For this analysis, we calculated the amount of time par-
ticipants spent in these more worthwhile ways (Kuykendall et al.,
2018): on social (vs. solo) discretionary activities, active (vs. pas-
sive) discretionary activities, and productive (vs. nonproductive)
discretionary activities.
To determine which uses of discretionary time most people con-
sider to be social (vs. solo), active (vs. passive), and productive
(vs. nonproductive), we again conducted a crowdsourcing study
on Amazon Mechanical Turk among a separate set of participants
(N = 901; ages 1874, M
age
= 37.55; 60.7% male). In this preregis-
tered study, we provided the same denition of discretionary time
and presented participants with the discretionary activities identi-
ed in our previous crowdsourcing study (see Table S8S10). To
reduce the chance of respondent fatigue, each participant was pre-
sented with a random subset of 31 activities from the full list of 94
discretionary activities. For each discretionary activity, partici-
pants indicated whether it was social versus solo (A social activ-
ity is one that would be more likely to involve engaging with other
people (vs. alone);0=solo activity,1=social activity); whether
it was active versus passive (An active activity is one that is
physically or mentally engaging. The opposite of an active activity
is a passive activity;0=passive activity,1=active activity) and
whether it was productive versus nonproductive (By productive,
we mean that you consider this use of discretionary time to not be
wasted. This use of time might feel useful, accomplished, fulll-
ing, helpful, purposeful, and/or worthwhile;0=not productive,
1=productive).
As preregistered, we coded discretionary activities to be social,
active, or productive if 90% or more of the participants rated the
activity as such. Otherwise, the activity was coded as solo, pas-
sive, or nonproductive. See Tables S8S10 in the online supple-
mental materials for a complete list of the activities and the
percentage of participants who categorized them as social, active,
and productive. Notably, this crowdsourcing study revealed a large
degree of overlap between the discretionary activities that were
viewed as active and productive, with all but one of the productive
activities (i.e., Hobbies) also identied as active. These results,
along with our rationale that productive discretionary time is likely
to be worthwhile by being physically or mentally engaging, led us
to combine these two categories into discretionary activities that
are productive (vs. nonproductive).
6
Based on this classication process, the discretionary activities
counted as social were Socializing and Communication with
Others, Playing Sports with Household Children, Playing Volley-
ball, Playing Football, Playing Racquet Sports, and Playing Bil-
liards. The discretionary activities we counted as productive were
Working Out, Running, Hobbies, Bowling, Participating in Water
Sports, Playing Volleyball, Playing Rugby, Participating in Eques-
trian Sports, Playing Baseball, Weightlifting/Strength Training,
Biking, Playing Sports with Household or Non-Household
Figure 3
Histogram of the Relationship Between Discretionary Time and Subjective Well-Being in Study 2
6.4
6.5
6.6
6.7
6.8
6.9
7
7.1
7.2
7.3
7.4
30 min
1 hr
1.5 hr
2 hr
2.5 hr
3 hr
3.5 hr
4 hr
4.5 hr
5 hr
5.5 hr
6 hr
6.5 hr
7 hr
7.5 hr
8 hr
8.5 hr
9 hr
9.5 hr
10 hr
10.5 hr
11 hr
11.5 hr
12 hr
12.5 hr
13 hr
13.5 hr
14 hr
14.5 hr
Subjecitve Well-Being
Discretionary Time
Note. Discretionary time is split into bins of 30 minutes. Error bars represent standard error of the mean.
6
The pattern of the results is consistent when the active and productive
categorizations are kept separate. See the online supplemental materials for
the separated results.
DISCRETIONARY TIME AND SUBJECTIVE WELL-BEING
7
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Children, Doing Aerobics, Participating in Martial Arts, Personal/
Private Activities (e.g., making out, having sex), Rollerblading,
Wrestling, Fencing, Playing Hockey, Playing Soccer, Playing Rac-
quet Sports, and Golng. Using these classications, we calculated
the amount of time each participant in the ATUS spent on discre-
tionary activities that were social (vs. solo) and productive (vs.
nonproductive) that day.
For each type of discretionary time, we examined whether there
was a signicant negative quadratic effect by conducting OLS
regressions predicting subjective well-being from the linear and
quadratic term of each type of discretionary time. Following this,
we examined if there was a signicant inverted U-shape for each
type of discretionary time.
Social Versus Solo Discretionary Time
For solo discretionary time, we found a signicant negative
quadratic effect (N = 21,736, B = .003, SE = .001, t[21733] =
4.04, p , .001, 95% CI [.005, .002], b = .079, R
2
= .004;
with covariates: N = 20,275, B = .003, SE = .001, t[20264] =
3.87, p , .001, 95% CI [.005, .002], b = .078, R
2
= .032).
Simonsohns (2018) two-lines approach conrmed an inverted
U-shape. For low values of solo discretionary time, the relation-
ship with subjective well-being was positive and statistically sig-
nicant (b = .05, z = 1.91, p = .056); however, for high values of
solo discretionary time, the regression line was negative and statis-
tically signicant (b = .05, z = 7.59, p , .001; break point =
2.53 hr).
However, for social discretionary time, we did not nd a signi-
cant negative quadratic effect (N = 21,736, B = .003, SE = .003, t
[21733] = 1.11, p = .269, 95% CI [.008, .002], b = .015, R
2
=
.001; with covariates: N = 20,275, B = .000, SE = .003, t[20264] =
.012, p = .991, 95% CI [.005, .005], b = .000, R
2
= .031). This
nonsignicant quadratic effect was thus signicantly smaller than
for solo discretionary time activities, comparing the standardized
coefcients of the quadratics,
7
F(1, 21731) = 8.26. We instead only
found a signicant positive linear effect of social discretionary time
on life satisfaction (N = 21,736, B = .033, SE = .009, t[21734] =
3.63, p , .001, 95% CI [.015, .052], R
2
= .001; with covariates:
N = 20,275, B = .041, SE = .009, t[20265] = 4.36, p , .001, 95% CI
[.023, .059], R
2
= .030).
Together these results suggest that when people spend their dis-
cretionary time socially, more is better. We only observed the too
much time effect when that discretionary time did not offer the
value of social connection.
Productive Versus Nonproductive Discretionary time
For nonproductive discretionary time, we found a signicant
negative quadratic effect (N = 21,736, B = .004, SE = .001,
t[21733] = 4.67, p , .001, 95% CI [.005, .002], b = .095,
R
2
= .004; with covariates: N = 20,275, B = .003, SE = .001,
t[20264] = 3.91, p , .001, 95% CI [.005, .002], b = .082,
R
2
= .032). Furthermore, we found evidence for the inverted
U-shape. For low values of nonproductive discretionary time, the
regression line was positive and statistically signicant (b = .059,
z = 2.43, p = .015); and for high values of nonproductive discre-
tionary time, the regression line was negative and statistically sig-
nicant (b = .044, z = 7.49, p , .001; break point = 3 hr).
For productive discretionary time, we also found a signicant
negative quadratic effect (N = 21,736, B = .016, SE = .007,
t[21733] = 2.36, p = .018, 95% CI [.030, .003], b = .030,
R
2
= .002; with covariates: N = 20,275, B = .016, SE = .007,
t[20264] = 2.27, p = .023, 95% CI [.030, .002], b = .029,
R
2
= .033). However, the negative quadratic effect was signi-
cantly smaller for productive (vs. nonproductive) uses of discre-
tionary time, comparing the standardized coefcients of the
quadratics, F(1, 21731) = 6.16 (p = .013).
8
Furthermore, we did
not nd evidence for the inverted U-shape. For low values of pro-
ductive discretionary time, the regression line was positive and
statistically signicant (b = 4.09, z = 2.41, p = .016); however, for
high values of productive discretionary time, the regression line
did not reach statistical signicance (b = .032, z = 1.35, p = .176;
break point = .22 hr).
Altogether, this exploration into types of discretionary time sug-
gests that how people spend their time does affect the relationship
between the amount of discretionary time people have and their
subjective well-being. In particular, the manner in which people
spend their discretionary time appears to largely determine
whether we observe the negative effect of having too much discre-
tionary time. Although an abundance of discretionary time spent
on solo and nonproductive activities did produce a negative effect
on subjective well-being, discretionary time spent on activities
that were social or productive did not. With an already vast litera-
ture asserting the emotional benets of social connection (Diener
& Seligman, 2002; Gable & Bromberg, 2018), we were particu-
larly interested in the moderating role of discretionary activities
that were productive (vs. nonproductive), revealing that only when
people spent large amounts of discretionary time nonproductively
did they report lower subjective well-being. In light of prior work
showing that people enjoy increased satisfaction from feeling pro-
ductive and busy (Etkin & Mogilner, 2016; Hsee et al., 2010), this
nding suggests that a lacking sense of productivity may be one
underlying mechanism driving the negative effect of having too
much time. We further examine this nding in the subsequent two
studies.
Study 3: Discretionary Time Experiment
Even though our analyses of the large nationally representative
data sets in Studies 1 and 2 controlled for likely covariates (e.g.,
7
To compute this statistic, we z-scored each of the following terms: amount
of discretionary time people spent on solo activities, amount of discretionary
time people spent on social activities, the amount of discretionary time people
had on solo activities squared, the amount of discretionary time people had on
social activities squared, and life satisfaction. We then conducted a linear
regression predicting life satisfaction from this set of predictor variables. The
statistic examines whether the quadratic terms for social vs. solo discretionary
time are statistically different from each other in this regression model.
8
To compute this statistic, we z-scored each of the following terms:
amount of discretionary time people spent on productive activities, amount
of discretionary time people spent on nonproductive activities, the amount
of discretionary time people spent on productive activities squared, the
amount of discretionary time people spent on nonproductive activities
squared, and life satisfaction. We then conducted a linear regression
predicting life satisfaction from this set of predictor variables. The statistic
examines whether the quadratic terms for productive vs. nonproductive
discretionary time are statistically different from each other in this
regression model.
8
SHARIF, MOGILNER, AND HERSHFIELD
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
employment status, marital and parental status, income, etc.), it
remains plausible that inherent differences between people with
low, moderate, and high amounts of discretionary time account for
their differing levels of subjective well-being. For instance,
because we did not have data on mental health status, it is possible
that individuals with depressive symptoms and who are dissatis-
ed with life may also be unmotivated to fulll their obligations,
which would leave them with large amounts of discretionary time.
To address such alternate explanations, in Study 3, we randomly
assigned participants to the experience of having a low, moderate,
or high amount of discretionary time.
Study 3 thus employed a between-subjects experimental design.
Because it is infeasible to manipulate the actual number of discre-
tionary and nondiscretionary hours individuals typically have in their
day-to-day lives, we relied on a mental simulation manipulation that
required participants to vividly imagine and describe having a given
number of discretionary hours every day for an extended period of
their lives. This mental simulation approach helped ensure that par-
ticipants considered the nuanced and actual experience of being in
that situation, rather than reporting based on their supercial assump-
tions (Morewedge et al., 2010; OBrien et al., 2018).
In the previous studies, we had to rely on the measures that
were available in those data sets. In Studies 1 and 2, subjective
well-being was measured using items that assessed satisfaction in
life. However, life satisfaction is just one component of subjective
well-being, and it is one that is more cognitive in nature (Kahne-
man & Deaton, 2010). In these next studies, we used a more com-
prehensive measure of subjective well-being that encompasses
both life satisfaction and positive emotion (Diener et al., 2017;
Lyubomirsky et al., 2005).
Perhaps most important, Study 3 sought to illuminate the under-
lying mechanism for the negative quadratic effect observed in Stud-
ies 1 and 2. In light of previous work showing the detrimental
emotional consequences of feeling time-stressed (Kasser & Shel-
don, 2009; Roxburgh, 2004), we proposed that compared with hav-
ing a moderate amount of discretionary time, having a small
amount of discretionary time would make people feel more stressed
and thus experience lower subjective well-being. On the other end
of the continuum, in light of the moderating role of discretionary
time spent productively (vs. nonproductively) in Study 2, as well as
work showing the benets of being productive (Etkin & Mogilner,
2016; Hsee et al., 2010; Keinan & Kivetz, 2011), we proposed that
compared with having a moderate amount of discretionary time,
having too much discretionary time would make people feel unpro-
ductive and thus experience lower subjective well-being.
Method
As preregistered, 2,550 American adults were recruited on Ama-
zons Mechanical Turk to participate in this study.
9
Two thousand,
ve hundred sixty-ve participants (M
age
= 37.83; 40.9% male;
58.1% employed; 47.7% married; 50.6% have children; 69.2% with
at least a bachelor's degree; M
HoursWork/Week =
30.26 hours
10
;
M
DiscretionaryTime/Day =
3.54
11
hr) completed the three-cell (amount
of discretionary time: low vs. moderate vs. high) between-subjects
experiment.
After being presented with the denition of discretionary time,
time spent on leisure activities or on other pursuits where the pri-
mary function is the use of time for pleasure or some other
intrinsically worthwhile purpose, participants were led to men-
tally simulate having a given amount of discretionary time every
day for at least 6 months of their lives. Participants were randomly
assigned to have a low (15 minutes per day), moderate (3.5 hr per
day), or high (7 hr per day) amount of discretionary time.
12
We
specied that they would have this amount of time consistently
over a minimum six-month period to ensure that participants in
the high time condition did not imagine a single vacation day or
weekend day that was uncharacteristically relaxed, and that partic-
ipants in the low time condition did not imagine a single workday
that was uncharacteristically busy.
For each condition, participants were prompted to imagine and
vividly describe what it would be like to have the given amount of
discretionary time (e.g., how they would spend this time, what
they would be doing during the other portion of the day, and how
they would be feeling that day) after 1 day, 1 month, 3 months,
and 6 months of having that amount of discretionary time every
day.
Participants then reported their subjective well-being by rating
the extent to which they would experience enjoyment, happiness,
and satisfaction on a 7-point Likert scale, with 1 = would not feel
this at all to 7 = would feel this a lot (a
low
= .96, a
moderate
= .94,
a
high
= .94). To test our proposed mechanisms, we then asked par-
ticipants to rate on the same 7-point scale their stress (stressful,
rushed, exhausting; a
low
= .91, a
moderate
= .91, a
high
= .88) and
their sense of productivity (productive, purposeful, I accomplished
a lot; a
low
= .90, a
moderate
= .91, a
high
= .93) during this period of
their lives.
Lastly, participants were asked a series of questions capturing
features of their imagined time period: employment status, the
number of hours worked per day, occupation, marital status, pa-
rental status, whether on vacation, whether in-between jobs, and
whether they had experienced any signicant life changes right
before this period of their life.
All materials, data, syntax, and preregistrations for both Study 3
and 4 can be found at https://osf.io/hw85m/. Both Studies 3 and 4
were conducted with Institutional Review Board approval from
the University of California, Los Angeles under protocol IRB#
16000935-AM-00006, Time, Money, and Happiness.
Results
The results of this experiment were c onsistent with the nega -
tive quadratic effect observed in Studies 1 and 2. We conducted
an OLS regression predicting subjective well-being from two
dummy variables representing conditio ns, with the moderate
discretionary time co ndition serving as the reference group.
Participants r eported lo wer subjec tive well-bei ng from ha ving a
9
The sample size was determined based on a power analysis of a pilot
of this study.
10
Two participants reported working more hours than the number of
hours in a week and were excluded from this statistic.
11
One hundred fty-four participants reported more than 24 hours of
discretionary time in a day and were excluded from this statistic.
12
These amounts were determined based on the data from Study 2. The
moderate amount (3.5 hours) was set to be halfway between the identied
range of optimal discretionary time (between 2 and 5 hours). The high
amount of discretionary time was set to be double this amount of time (7
hours).
DISCRETIONARY TIME AND SUBJECTIVE WELL-BEING
9
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
low amount of discretionary time than from having a moderate
amount of discretionary time (M
Low
=5.52, SD = 1.55 vs. M
Moderate
=
6.00, SD =1.14;B = .480, t[2562] = 7.36, p , .001, 95% CI
[.607, .352], d = .35), and participants reported lower subjective
well-being from having a high amount of discretionary time than
from having a moderate amount of discretio nary time (M
High
=5.88,
SD =1.30vs.M
Moderate
=6.00,SD =1.14;B = .119, t[ 2562] =
1.84, p = .066, 95% CI [.247, .008], d = .10). This effect held
when controlling for characteristics of the imagined time period,
including employment status (employed or not), number of working
hours,
13
marital status (married or not), parental status (have children
or not), whether they imagined being on a vacation or not, whether
they imagined being between jobs or not, and whether they imagined
there was any signicant life change right before the imagined time
period or not (Low vs. Moderate Discretionary Time: B = .463, t
[2554] = 7.16, p , .001, 95% CI [.590, .337]; High vs. Moder-
ate Discretionary Time: B = .152, t[2554] = 2.35, p =.02,95%CI
[.279, .025]).
We also found support for our proposed mechanisms. Participants
reported they would feel signicantly more stress from having a low
amount of discretionary time than from having a moderate amount
(M
Low
= 3.24, SD =1.89vs.M
Moderate
= 2.56, SD =1.62;B = .673,
t[2562] = 8.17, p , .001, 95% CI [.511, .835], d = .38); and these
feelings of stress partially mediated the negative effect of having a
low (vs. moderate) amount of discretionary time on subjective well-
being (B = .26, SE = .04, 95% CI [.339, .193])basedon10,000
bootstrap samples with a reduced direct effect (B = .22, SE = .06,
t[2562] = 3.78, p , .001, 95% CI [.330, .104]). Additionally,
participants reported they would feel signicantly less productive
having a high amount of discretionary time than having a moderate
amount (M
High
=5.11,SD =1.71vs.M
Moderate
=5.31,SD =1.48;B =
.20, t[2562] = 2.64, p , .01, 95% CI [.355, .052], d =.13),
and this lacking sense of productivity signica ntly mediated the nega-
tive effect of having a high (vs. moderate) amount of discretionary
time on subjective well-being (B = .09, SE = .04, 95% CI [.167,
.024]) based on 10,000 bootstrap samples with no signicant direct
effect (B = .03, SE =.05,t[2562] = .464, 95% CI [.131, .081]).
Study 3 revealed that a lacking sense of productivity is one
mechanism explaining the negative effect on subjective well-being
from having a large (vs. moderate) amount of discretionary time.
Along with the moderating role of productive (vs. nonproductive)
discretionary activities in Study 2, these results suggest that the
negative effect of having too much discretionary time would be
attenuated when that discretionary time is spent productively. We
tested this in the next study.
Study 4: Productive Versus Nonproductive
Discretionary Time
In Study 4, we focus on the negative effect of having too much
discretionary time for subjective well-being. In particular, based
on our ndings in Studies 2 and 3, we predicted that if people
spent their discretionary time nonproductively, they would report
lower subjective well-being with high (vs. moderate) amounts of
discretionary time. However, if they instead spent their discretion-
ary time on productive activities, the negative effect of having a
high (vs. moderate) amount of discretionary time would be attenu-
ated. Study 4 thus sought to provide additional evidence for the
exploratory analyses in Study 2 using random assignment in a
between-subjects experimental design.
Method
As preregistered, 5,000 American adults
14
were recruited on
Amazons Mechanical Turk to participate in this study. Five thou-
sand and one participants (ages 1891, M
age
= 38.30; 50.3%
male
15
) completed this 2 (amount of discretionary time: moderate
vs. high) 3 2 (type of discretionary time: productive vs. nonpro-
ductive) between-subjects experiment.
Participants were asked to mentally simulate having either 3.5 hr
of discretionary time (moderate discretionary time condition) or 7
hr of discretionary time (high discretionary time condition) for at
least 6 months of their life. They were presented with a revised and
more straightforward denition of discretionary time: time spent
on activities that are pleasurable or meaningful to you.
16
In addi-
tion, participants were instructed to imagine spending this time on
productive [nonproductive] activities: you consider this use of dis-
cretionary time to NOT be wasted [to be wasted]. This use of time
would feel [would NOT feel] useful, accomplished, fullling, help-
ful, purposeful, and/or worthwhile. We specied that they would
have this amount of time consistently over at least a six-month pe-
riod, and this is how they would spend it.
For each condition, participants were prompted to imagine and
vividly describe what it would be like to have the given amount of
discretionary time (i.e., how they would spend this time, what they
would be doing during the other portion of their days, and how they
would be feeling each day) throughout this period of their life.
Participants then reported their subjective well-being by rating the
extent to which they would experience enjoyment, happiness, and
satisfaction on a 7-point Likert scale, with 1 = would not feel this at
all to7=would feel this a lot (a
moderate, productive
= .90, a
moderate, not
productive
=.95;a
high, productive
= .91, a
high, not productive
= .96).
Last, participants were asked a series of questions capturing fea-
tures of their imagined time period: employment status, the number
of hours worked per day, occupation, marital status, parental status,
whether on vacation, whether in between jobs, and whether they had
experienced any signicant life changes right before this period of
their life. They were also asked manipulation check questions about
the amount of discretionary time they imagined having (During the
period of your life that you previously described, how much discre-
tionary time did you have? on a 11-point scale with 5=very little
discretionary time and5=a lot of discretionary time) and about
how productively they imagined using this time (To what extent
would you describe this period of your life as the following? for
productive,”“purposeful, and I accomplished a lot on 7-point
Likert scales with 1 = would not feel this at all and 7 = would feel
this a lot; a
moderate, productive
= .88, a
moderate, not productive
=.95;a
high,
productive
=.88,a
high, not productive
=.96).
13
One participant reported imagining working more than 24 hours a day
and was excluded from this analysis.
14
This sample size was determined by a power analysis based on Study 3.
15
Because of an experimenter error, we did not collect information on
additional participant demographics.
16
This denition was suggested by one of the reviewers during this
papers review process.
10
SHARIF, MOGILNER, AND HERSHFIELD
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Results
Eight hundred fteen participants failed the attention check. Of
those who passed the attention check, 140 participants wrote
meaningless/nonsensical responses when describing how they
would spend their discretionary time. As preregistered, we
excluded these participants, leaving a nal sample of 4,046 partici-
pants (ages 1891, M
age
= 38.51; 46.3% male). First, conrming
our manipulation check, participants considered their time to be
more productive in the productive conditions than the nonproduc-
tive conditions (M
Productive
= 6.12, SD = .98 vs. M
NotProductive
=3.31, SD = 1.94; t[4044] = 58.21, p , .001, 95% CI [2.89,
2.71]). Participants also perceived they had more discretionary
time in the high versus moderate amount of discretionary time
conditions (M
High
= 9.67, SD = 1.86 vs. M
Moderate
=8.76, SD =
2.01; t[4044] = 14.90, p , .001, 95% CI [1.03, .789]).
We conducted an OLS regression predicting subjective well-
being from a dummy variable representing the amount of discre-
tionary time (high vs. moderate), a dummy variable representing
the type of discretionary time (productive vs. nonproductive), and
a variable representing their interaction. As predicted, we found a
signicant 2 (amount of discretionary time: high vs. moderate) x 2
(type of discretionary time: productive vs. nonproductive) interac-
tion (B = .514, SE = .089, t[4042] = 5.76, p , .001, 95% CI [.339,
.689]).
17,18
Simple effect analysis revealed that when people spent
their discretionary time nonproductively, people reported lower
subjective well-being when they had a high (7 hr) versus moderate
(3.5 hr) amount of discretionary time (M
High
= 4.90, SD= 1.84 vs.
M
Moderate
= 5.30, SD = 1.66; B = .400, SE = .064, t[4042] =
6.27, p , .001, 95% CI [.524, .275], d = .23; Figure 4).
However, when people spent their time productively, they reported
marginally higher subjective well-being when they had a high (7
hr) versus moderate (3.5 hr) amount of discretionary time (M
High
=
6.20, SD = .98 vs. M
Moderate
= 6.08, SD = 1.02; B = .114, SE =
.063, t[4042] = 1.83, p = .068, 95% CI [.008, .237], d = .11; Fig-
ure 4).
This interaction held when controlling for characteristics of
their imagined time period: employment status, number of work-
ing hours,
19
marital status, parental status, whether on vacation,
whether in-between jobs, and whether there had been any signi-
cant life change right before, all of which were mean-centered
(B = .482, SE = .088, t[4031] = 5.47, p , .001, 95% CI [.309,
.655]).
20
Simple effect analysis revealed that when people spent
their discretionary time nonproductively, they reported lower sub-
jective well-being when they had a high (7 hr) versus moderate
(3.5 hr) amount of discretionary time (B = .378, SE = .064,
t[4031] = 5.94, p , .001, 95% CI [.503, .253]). However,
when people spent their discretionary time productively, they
reported a marginally higher subjective well-being when they had
a high (7 hr) versus moderate (3.5 hr) amount of discretionary
time (B = .104, SE = .062, t[4031] = 1.67, p = .095, 95% CI
[.018, .227]).
As in the exploratory analyses in Study 2, Study 4 further dem-
onstrates that the type of discretionary time moderates the effect of
amount of discretionary time on subjective well-being. In particu-
lar, productive uses of discretionary time attenuate the negative
effect of having too much discretionary time.
General Discussion
In two large-scale nationally representative data sets and two
experiments, we investigated the relationship between the
amount of di scretionary time people have in their daily lives and
their subjective well-being. Leveraging larger sample sizes and
more objectiv e assess men t s of tim e afuence, these results sub-
stantiate the previously observed emotional burden of feeling
time-constrained (Hamermesh & Lee, 2007; Kasser & Sheldon,
2009). We fo und that having a dearth o f discre tionary hours in
ones day indeed results in greater stress and lower subjective
well-being. Although too little time is bad, we further found that
having more time is not a lways better. In fact, t he resu lts fro m
Studies 24 suggest it is possible to have too much time. With
prior literatures having separately examined the negative effects
of being too busy (e.g., Kasser & Sheldon, 2009; Roxburgh,
2004) or not being busy enough (e.g., Bellezza et al., 2017 ;
Hsee et al., 2010; Keinan & Kivetz, 2011), ou r research is
among the rst to bring these perspectives together. By testing
across the full range of daily discretionary hours, our ndings
clarify the suboptimal emotional impact of having either too lit-
tle or too much time.
Because discretionary time is the amount of time one spends on
activities that are pleasurable or intrinsically rewarding (i.e., activ-
ities one wants to do), how is it possible to have too much? In line
with the Aristotelian urging against excess and toward moderation,
as well as mounting evidence for the possibility of having too
much of [any] good thing (Grant & Schwartz, 2011), our ndings
indicate that an overabundance of discretionary hours in ones
days can too be associated with lower subjective well-being. The
attenuating role of whether peoples discretionary time was spent
productively (Studies 2 and 4), along with the reduced feelings of
productivity reported among those who had been allocated a high
amount of discretionary time (Study 3), point to an explanation.
These results suggest that having too much discretionary time
undermines peoples sense of productivity and purpose, thus leav-
ing them less satised overall.
Although our investigation centered on the relationship
between amount of discretionary time and subjective well-
being, our additional exploration into how individuals spend
their discretionary time proved revealing. In particular, we
17
Preregistered as an exploratory analysis, examining just the
satisfaction item as the dependent variable similarly showed a signicant
interaction (B =.510, SE = .096, t[4042] = 5.30, p , .001, 95% CI [.321,
.698]).
18
Although qualied by the signicant interaction, the results also
showed a signicant negative main effect of having a high (vs. moderate)
amount of discretionary time on subjective well-being (B = .138, SE =
.045, t[4043] = 3.08, p , .001, 95% CI [.226, .050]), as well as a
signicant positive main effect of productive (vs. nonproductive) use of
that time on subjective well-being (B = 1.03, SE = .045, t(4043) = 23.08,
p , .001, 95% CI [.946, 1.122]).
19
Four participants reported imagining working more than 24 hours a
day and were thus excluded from this analysis.
20
Although qualied by the signicant interaction, the results also
showed a signicant negative main effect of having a high (vs. moderate)
amount of discretionary time on subjective well-being (B = .134, SE =
.045, t[4043] = 2.97, p , .001, 95% CI [.222, .045]), as well as a
signicant positive main effect of productive (vs. nonproductive) use of
that time on subjective well-being (B = .977, SE = .045, t[4043] = 21.89,
p , .001, 95% CI [.889, 1.06]).
DISCRETIONARY TIME AND SUBJECTIVE WELL-BEING
11
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
found that if people spent their discretionary time in worthwhile
wayson productive or social activitiesthe negative effect of
having too much time was attenuated. Thus, if the particular
way people spend their discretionary t ime is not accounted for
(as in Study 1 and Study 3), the detrimental effect of having an
overabundance may be weak or nonexistent. These ndings sup-
port self-determin atio n theorys assertion that well-being
requires fulllment of three fundamental psychological needs:
autonomy, relatedness, and competence (e.g., Sheldo n et al.,
2010; Sheldon & Niemiec, 2006 ). With having discretion in
what activities one does likely contributing to a sense of
autonomy, engaging in social activities likely contributing to a
sense of relatedness, and engaging in productive activities likely
contributing to a sense of competence, our research importantly
highlights that the number of daily hours an individual has
available to spend as they choose, as well as how they allocate
those hours, is critical to well-being.
These ndings also have clear practical implications for indi-
vidualsparticularly those who experience t ime poverty. For the
many who feel unhappy from the stress of having too much to do
and not enough time to do it, the answer is not to quit all obliga-
tions. Our ndings suggest that ending up with entire days free
to ll at ones discretion may leave one similarly unhappy. Fig-
ure 3 from Study 2, for instance, suggests that beyond having
two hours of discretionary time in the day, having more time
does not promise greater happiness. That gure a lso suggests
that beyond approximately ve hours of discretionary time in the
day, having more is linked to less happiness. Thus, the time poor
should not quit everything and neglect all of their obligations;
instead, they should strive for the reasonably attainable amount
of having a little over two hours to spend how they want during
their days. To be clear though, these amounts are inexact and
based merely on eyeballing a graph, which represents one dataset
and buckets together many types of individuals according to the
amount of time each spent on discretionary activities in one
given day. However, t he overall inverted U-shaped pattern is ro-
bust across people and does offer useful general guidance.
Namely, the signicant negative quadratic relationship identied
across multiple large data sets representing the diversity of
American adults, as well as the nonsignicant interaction effects
comparing segments of the population, preliminarily suggest that
whether young or old, working or unemployed, male or
female, married or single, with chi ldren or withoutmost would
benet from having a moderate amount of discretionary time:
not too little and not too much.
In cases when individuals do nd themselves in circumstances
with excessive amounts of discretionary time (e.g., upon retire-
ment or having left a job), our results suggest these individuals
would benet from spending their newfound time with purpose (e.
g., productively or connecting with others). These ndings are
consistent with prior research conducted among retirees (a popula-
tion with ample discretionary time), showing that retirees well-
being depends on their level of social interaction (Longino & Kart,
1982) and participation in clubs or volunteer work (Kelly & Ross,
1989; Mishra, 1992; Morrow-Howell, 2010).
Limitations and Directions for Future Research
Although robust, it is worth noting that the observed effect of
discretionary time on subjective well-being is small. However, we
would not expect the quantity of ones hours spent on discretion-
ary activities in a day to have any larger of an effect on subjective
well-being than we observed. This is because, in addition to other
time-related variables that inuence subjective well-being (e.g.,
how people spend their hours, Kahneman et al., 2004 ; their mind-
set during those hours, Killingsworth & Gilbert, 2010; West et al.,
Figure 4
Means of Subjective Well-Being by Condition in Study 4
3.5
4
4.5
5
5.5
6
6.5
Nonproductive Productive
Subjective Well-Being
Moderate High
Note. Error bars represent standard error of the mean.
12
SHARIF, MOGILNER, AND HERSHFIELD
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
2021; and how their obligatory time is lled, Judge & Shinichiro,
1993); there are a slew of other variables that play into peoples
overall assessment of their satisfaction in life (Lyubomirsky et al.,
2005). Furthermore, despite it being small, we are condent in its
existence and shape. Our predicted negative quadratic effect repli-
cated across data sets and subsets of the data sets, and it persisted
with and without the inclusion of relevant covariates. The negative
effect of having too much discretionary time is particularly small,
which we found is partly explained by its sensitivity to the particu-
lar ways that time is spent. Albeit small, together these ndings
provide preliminary theoretically and practically important
insights about the role of discretionary time allocation and usage
for well-being.
As an initial exploration into the relationship between time
afuence and subjective well-being, our hope is that this work
spurs future investigations into more precise research questions.
For instance, the current ndings examine how the amount of dis-
cretionary time a person has on a typical day relates to well-being,
but it does not inform the experience of atypical days, like when
on vacation or on a holiday. Furthermore, although we have pro-
vided initial insight into possible mechanisms (i.e., stress for too
little time and lack of productivity for too much time), our identi-
ed effect is likely multiply determined. For example, boredom
may also contribute to reduced well-being from having an over-
abundance of discretionary time. Thus, it is possible that factors
that have been previously identied as effective in offsetting
hedonic adaptation (e.g., variety across activities; Etkin & Mogil-
ner, 2016; or taking breaks between enjoyable experiences; Quoid-
bach & Dunn, 2013) may be similarly effective in informing
people how to optimally allocate their discretionary time across a
day or week.
Additionally, building off of our proposed mechanism, we
tested a few ways in which our effect might be attenuated (i.e.,
spending discretionary time on worthwhile endeavors, such as
activities that are productive or socially connecting). However,
there are a number of additional ways to characterize the way peo-
ple spend their discretionary time that might also inuence our
observed relationship. We hope future research investigates the
psychological and behavioral nuances within the category of dis-
cretionary activities.
Future work should also experimentally examine the precise point
at which an abundance of discretionary time starts to negatively
affect subjective well-being. The correlational data in Figure 3 sug-
gest that people are happiest having between two and ve ho ur s
of di sc re tion ar y time, and that the s lop e is neg at ive beyond that
point. The results from our experimental studies similarly show
that people with 7 hr of discretionary time experience less sub-
jective well-being than people with 3.5 hr of discretionary t ime.
However, futu re wo rk should experiment ally manipulate at a
more granular level the amount of discretionary time partici-
pants have to more precisely identify the optimal amount of dis-
cretionary time.
Relatedly, the results of Study 2 suggest that people can benet
from a greater amount of discretionary time before it negatively
affects their subjective well-being as long as the time is spent pro-
ductively or with others. Thus, future work should also identify
optimal amounts of discretionary time depending on how that time
is used, as well as how to optimally allocate given amounts of dis-
cretionary time between relaxation, socializing, and productive
use. Our data cannot speak to, for instance, whether a small
amount of discretionary time is better spent relaxing and unwind-
ing in front of the TV, or whether that limited spare time should
be diligently spent going for a run or knitting a sweater.
It is also possible that the relationship between amount of dis-
cretionary time and subjective well-being varies cross-culturally.
For example, although busyness is associated with higher social
status in the United States ( Bellezza et al., 2017), this may not be
the case in other countries that place greater value on relaxation
(Brislin & Kim, 2003). Without the same compulsion toward pro-
ductivity (Mogilner & Norton, 2019), the negative effect of having
too much discretionary time that we identied in our American
samples may not extend more broadly.
People often complain about being too busy and express want-
ing more time (Trupia et al., 2021). However, Studies 3 and 4 sug-
gest that when people mentally simulate the experience of having
an abundance of discretionary time, they are able to forecast its
detrimental effects when spent unproductively. Although prior
research has validated the veracity of mental simulation in captur-
ing actual experience (Morewedge et al., 2010; OBrien et al.,
2018), future research should examine when people are able to
correctly forecast that an abundance of discretionary time would
have a negative impact on their well-being. For example, when
people are simply asked (without being encouraged to mentally
simulate the experience), do they think that having more discre-
tionary time would always make them happier?
Other open questions involve the role of choice and whether
there is an end point to the period of time in which people have an
ample amount of discretionary time during their days. For
instance, would we observe the same pattern of results among
those on a sabbatical (for whom it was a choice, and there is an
endpoint) and for those who decided to retire early (for whom it
was a choice, but there is no endpoint)? Even though the results
from Study 2 persisted for those in and out of the workforce, and
even though the results from Studies 3 and 4 persisted when con-
trolling for whether participants were imagining being employed
or unemployed, it may be the case that if ones ample amount of
discretionary time is imposed (e.g., being laid off), the relationship
between too much discretionary time and subjective well-being
may be even more pronounced.
Lastly, although our pattern of results persisted across multiple
large-scale correlational data sets and two experiments, future ex-
perimental work is needed to test these ndings outside of a hypo-
thetical paradigm. Our causal evidence for how the amount of
discretionary time people have inuences their subjective well-
being relied on hypothetical mental simulations (Studies 3 and 4),
and
our manipulation for unproductive versus productive uses of
time (Study 4) was rather heavy-handed. Although resources lim-
ited our ability to actually assign very small or very large amounts
of discretionary time in peoples actual daily lives, we hope that
future experimentalists nd a way to test and conrm these nd-
ings among participants who actually live through the varied
conditions.
Conclusion
Despite our observed effect being small, the impact of time
afuence (or scarcity) for subjective well-being is important.
These ndings inform the ongoing investigation into the role of
DISCRETIONARY TIME AND SUBJECTIVE WELL-BEING
13
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
afuence as a signicant predictor of subjective well-beingbut
extend the focus from money (Kahneman & Deaton, 2010; Kill-
ingsworth, 2021) to include humans other principle resource:
time. This work thus contributes to the burgeoning literature on
time and subjective well-being (Mogilner et al., 2018) and adds a
qualication to research that has identied particularly happy
ways to spend time (Kahneman et al., 2004): solely lling ones
days with those activities may undermine feelings of purpose and
thus reduce satisfaction in life.
References
Banwell, C., Hinde, S., Dixon, J., & Sibthorpe, B. (2005). Reflections on
expert consensus: A case study of the social trends contributing to obe-
sity. European Journal of Public Health, 15(6), 564568. https://doi
.org/10.1093/eurpub/cki034
Bellezza, S., Paharia, N., & Keinan, A. (2017). Conspicuous consumption
of time: When busyness and lack of leisure time become a status sym-
bol. The Journal of Consumer Research, 44(1), 118138.
Brislin, R. W., & Kim, E. S. (2003). Cultural diversity in peoples under-
standing and uses of time. Applied Psychology, 52(3), 363382. https://
doi.org/10.1111/1464-0597.00140
Carroll, J. (2008). Time pressures, stress common for Americans. Gallup
Poll Social Series. http://www.gallup.com/poll/103456/Time-Pressures
-stress-Common-Americans.aspx
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). The costs and benefits of consuming The
Journal of Consumer Research, 27(2), 267272. https://doi.org/10.1086/
314324
Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Hunter, J. (2003). Happiness in everyday life:
The uses of experience sampling. Journal of Happiness Studies, 4(2),
185199. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1024409732742
Diener, E., Heintzelman, S., Kushlev, K., Tay, L., Wirtz, D., Lutes, L., &
Oishi, S. (2017). Findings all psychologists should know from the new
science on subjective well-being. Canadian Psychology, 58(2), 87104.
https://doi.org/10.1037/cap0000063
Diener, E., & Seligman, M. E. (2002). Very happy people. Psychological
Science, 13(1), 8184. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00415
Eriksson, L., Rice, J. M., & Goodin, R. E. (2007). Temporal aspects of life
satisfaction. Social Indicators Research, 80(3), 511533. https://doi.org/
10.1007/s11205-006-0005-z
Etkin, J., & Mogilner, C. (2016). Does variety among activities increase
happiness? The Journal of Consumer Research, 43(2), 210229. https://
doi.org/10.1093/jcr/ucw021
Frederick, S., & Loewenstein, G. (1999). Hedonic Adaptation. In D.
Kahneman, E. Diener, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Well-being: The founda-
tions of hedonic psychology (pp. 302329).
Russell Sage Foundation.
Gable, S. L., & Bromberg, C. (2018). Healthy social bonds: A necessary
condition for well- being. Handbook of well-being. DEF Publishers.
Galinsky, E., Bond, J. T., & Friedman, D. E. (1993). The changing work-
force: Highlights of the national study. DIANE Publishing.
Grant, A. M., & Schwartz, B. (2011). Too much of a good thing: The chal-
lenge and opportunity of the inverted U. Perspectives on Psychological
Science, 6(1), 6176. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691610393523
Hamermesh, D. S., & Lee, J. (2007). Stressed out on four continents: Time
crunch or yuppie kvetch? The Review of Economics and Statistics,
89(2), 374383. https://doi.org/10.1162/rest.89.2.374
Hershfield, H. E., Mogilner, C., & Barnea, U. (2016). People who choose
time over money are happier. Social Psychological & Personality Sci-
ence, 7(7), 697706. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550616649239
Holbrook, M. B., & Lehmann, D. R. (1981). Allocating discretionary time:
Complementarity among activities. The Journal of Consumer Research,
7(4), 395406. https://doi.org/10.1086/208830
Hsee, C. K., Yang, A. X., & Wang, L. (2010). Idleness aversion and the
need for justifiable busyness. Psychological Science, 21(7), 926930.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610374738
Judge, T. A., & Shinichiro, W. (1993). Another look at the job satisfac-
tion-life satisfaction relationship. Journal of Applied Psychology, 78(6),
939948. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.78.6.939
Kahneman, D., & Deaton, A. (2010). High income improves evaluation of
life but not emotional well-being. Proceedings of the National Academy
of Sciences of the United States of America, 107(38), 1648916493.
https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1011492107
Kahneman, D., Krueger, A. B., Schkade, D. A., Schwarz, N., & Stone,
A. A. (2004). A survey method for characterizing daily life experience:
The day reconstruction method. Science, 306(5702), 17761780. https://
doi.org/10.1126/science.1103572
Kasser, T., & Sheldon, K. M. (2009). Time affluence as a path toward per-
sonal happiness and ethical business practice: Empirical evidence from
four studies. Journal of Business Ethics, 84(S2), 243255. https://doi
.org/10.1007/s10551-008-9696-1
Keinan, A., & Kivetz, R. (2011). Productivity orientation and the con-
sumption of collectable experiences. The
Journal of Consumer
Research, 37(6), 935950. https://doi.org/10.1086/657163
Kelly, J. R., & Ross, J. E. (1989). Later-life leisure: Beginning a new
agenda. Leisure Sciences, 11(1), 4759. https://doi.org/10.1080/
01490408909512204
Killingsworth, M. A. (2021). Experienced well-being rises with income,
even above $75,000 per year. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences of the United States of America, 118(4), e2016976118. https://
doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2016976118
Killingsworth, M. A., & Gilbert, D. T. (2010). A wandering mind is an
unhappy mind. Science, 330(6006), 932. https://doi.org/10.1126/science
.1192439
Kurtz, J. L. (2008). Looking to the future to appreciate the present: The
benefits of perceived temporal scarcity. Psychological Science, 19(12),
12381241. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02231.x
Kushlev, K., Heintzelman, S. J., Oishi, S., & Diener, E. (2018). The declin-
ing marginal utility of social time for subjective well-being. Journal of
Research in Personality, 74, 124140. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2018
.04.004
Kuykendall, L., Boemerman, L., & Zhu, Z. (2018). The importance of lei-
sure for subjective well-being. In E. Diener, S. Oishi, & L. Tay (Eds.),
Handbook of well-being. DEF Publishers.
Lathia, N., Sandstrom, G. M., Mascolo, C., & Rentfrow, P. J. (2017). Hap-
pier people live more active lives: Using smartphones to link happiness
and physical activity. PLoS ONE, 12(1), e0160589. https://doi.org/10
.1371/journal.pone.0160589
Longino, C. F., Jr., & Kart, C. S. (1982). Explicating activity theory: A for-
mal replication. Journal of Gerontology, 37(6), 713722. https://doi.org/
10.1093/geronj/37.6.713
Lyubomirsky, S., Sheldon, K. M., & Schkade, D. (2005). Pursuing happi-
ness: The architecture of sustainable change. Review of General Psy-
chology, 9(2), 111131. https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.9.2.111
Masuda, Y. J., Williams, J. R., & Tallis, H. (2020). Does life satisfaction
vary with time and income? Investigating the relationship among free
time, income, and life satisfaction. Journal of Happiness Studies.
Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-020-00307-8
Mishra, S. (1992). Leisure activities and life satisfaction in old age: A case
study of retired government employees living in urban areas. Activities, Ad-
aptation and Aging, 16(4
), 726. https://doi.o rg/10.1300 /J016v16 n04_02
Mogilner, C. (2010). The pursuit of happiness: Time, money, and social
connection. Psychological Science, 21(9), 13481354. https://doi.org/10
.1177/0956797610380696
Mogilner, C., & Norton, M. (2019). Preferences for experienced versus
remembered happiness. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 14(2),
244251. https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2018.1460688
14
SHARIF, MOGILNER, AND HERSHFIELD
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Mogilner, C., Whillans, A., & Norton, M. I. (2018). Time, money, and sub-
jective well- being. Handbook of Well-Being. Noba Scholar Handbook
series: Subjective well-being. DEF Publishers.
Morewedge, C. K., Huh, Y. E., & Vosgerau, J. (2010). Thought for food:
Imagined consumption reduces actual consumption. Science, 330(6010),
15301533. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1195701
Morrow-Howell, N. (2010). Volunteering in later life: Research frontiers.
The Journals of Gerontology: Series B, 65(4), 461469. https://doi.org/
10.1093/geronb/gbq024
Newport, F. (2016). The Gallup poll: Public opinion 2015. Rowman & Lit-
tlefield Publishers.
OBrien, E., Kristal, A. C., Ellsworth, P. C., & Schwarz, N. (2018). (Mis)
imagining the good life and the bad life: Envy and pity as a function of
the focusing illusion. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 75,
4153. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2017.10.002
Quoidbach, J., & Dunn, E. W. (2013). Give it up: A strategy for combating
hedonic adaptation. Social Psychological & Personality Science, 4(5),
563568. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550612473489
Quoidbach, J., Dunn, E. W., Hansenne, M., & Bustin, G. (2015). The price
of abundance: How a wealth of experiences impoverishes savoring. Per-
sonality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 41(3), 393404. https://doi.org/
10.1177/0146167214566189
Roxburgh, S. (2004). There just arent enough hours in the day:Themental
health consequences of time pressure. Journal of Health and Social Behav-
ior, 45(2), 115131. https://doi.org/10.1177/002214650 40450020 1
Sheldon, K. M., Cummins, R., & Kamble, S. (2010). Life balance and
well-being: Testing a novel conceptual and measurement approach.
Journal of Personality, 78(4), 10931134. https://doi.org/10.1111/j
.1467-6494.2010.00644.x
Sheldon, K. M., & Niemiec, C. P. (2006). Its not just the amount that
counts: Balanced need satisfaction also affects well-being. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 91(2), 331341. https://doi.org/10
.1037/0022-3514.91.2.331
Simonsohn, U. (2018). Two lines: A valid alternative to the invalid testing
of
U-shaped relationships with quadratic regressions. Advances in Meth-
ods and Practices in Psychological Science, 1(4), 538555. https://doi
.org/10.1177/2515245918805755
Snir, R., & Zohar, D. (2008). Workaholism as discretionary time invest-
ment at work: An experience-sampling study. Applied Psychology,
57(1), 109127. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1464-0597.2006.00270.x
Strazdins, L., Griffin, A. L., Broom, D. H., Banwell, C., Korda, R., Dixon,
J., Paolucci, F., & Glover, J. (2011). Time scarcity: Another health in-
equality? Environment & Planning A, 43(3), 545559. https://doi.org/10
.1068/a4360
Teuchmann, K., Totterdell, P., & Parker, S. K. (1999). Rushed, unhappy,
and drained: An experience sampling study of relations between time
pressure, perceived control, mood, and emotional exhaustion in a group
of accountants. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 4(1),
3754. https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-8998.4.1.37
Trupia, M., Mogilner, C., & Engeler, I. (2021). Stressed or self-important?
Intentions versus perceptions when communicating busyness. Working
paper.
West, C., Mogilner, C., & DeVoe, S. (2021). Happiness from treating the
weekend like a vacation. Social Psychological & Personality Science,
12(3), 346356. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550620916080
Whillans, A. V., Dunn, E. W., Smeets, P., Bekkers, R., & Norton, M. I.
(2017). Buying time promotes happiness. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 114(32),
85238527. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1706541114
Whillans, A. V., Weidman , A. C., & Dunn, E. W. (2016) . Valuing time
over money is ass ociated with g reater h appines s. Social Psychologi-
cal & Personality Science, 7(3), 213222. https ://doi.org/ 10.1177/
1948550615623842
Wilson, T. D., & Gilbert, D. T. (2008). Explaining away: A model of affec-
tive adaptation. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3(5), 370386.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00085.x
Young, C., & Lim, C. (2014). Time as a network good: Evidence from
unemployment and the standard workweek. Sociological Science, 1,
1027. https://doi.org/10.15195/v1.a2
Received November 16, 2019
Revision received May 4, 2021
Accepted May 11, 2021
n
DISCRETIONARY TIME AND SUBJECTIVE WELL-BEING
15
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.